Trying to stay positive,
Skeptic
Darwin Was Wrong About Dating
By DAN SLATER
Published: January 12, 2013
A COUPLE of evolutionary psychologists recently published a book about
human sexual behavior in prehistory called “Sex at Dawn.” Upon hearing
of the project, one colleague, dubious that a modern scholar could hope
to know anything about that period, asked them, “So what do you do,
close your eyes and dream?”
Actually, it’s a little more involved. Evolutionary psychologists who
study mating behavior often begin with a hypothesis about how modern
humans mate: say, that men think about sex more than women do. Then they
gather evidence — from studies, statistics and surveys — to support
that assumption. Finally, and here’s where the leap occurs, they
construct an evolutionary theory to explain why men think about sex more
than women, where that gender difference came from, what adaptive
purpose it served in antiquity, and why we’re stuck with the
consequences today.
Lately, however, a new cohort of scientists have been challenging the
very existence of the gender differences in sexual behavior that
Darwinians have spent the past 40 years trying to explain and justify on
evolutionary grounds.
Of course, no fossilized record can really tell us how people behaved or thought back then, much less why
they behaved or thought as they did. Nonetheless, something funny
happens when social scientists claim that a behavior is rooted in our
evolutionary past. Assumptions about that behavior take on the
immutability of a physical trait — they come to seem as biologically
rooted as opposable thumbs or ejaculation.
Using evolutionary psychology to back up these assumptions about men and
women is nothing new. In “The Descent of Man, and Selection in Relation
to Sex,” Charles Darwin gathered evidence for the notion that, through
competition for mates and sustenance, natural selection had encouraged
man’s “more inventive genius” while nurturing woman’s “greater
tenderness.” In this way, he suggested that the gender differences he
saw around him — men sought power and made money; women stayed at home —
weren’t simply the way things were in Victorian England. They were the
way things had always been.
A century later, a new batch of scientists began applying Darwinian
doctrine to the conduct of mating, and specifically to three assumptions
that endure to this day: men are less selective about whom they’ll
sleep with; men like casual sex more than women; and men have more
sexual partners over a lifetime.
In 1972, Robert L. Trivers, a graduate student at Harvard, addressed
that first assumption in one of evolutionary psychology’s landmark
studies, “Parental Investment and Sexual Selection.”
He argued that women are more selective about whom they mate with
because they’re biologically obliged to invest more in offspring. Given
the relative paucity of ova and plenitude of sperm, as well as the
unequal feeding duties that fall to women, men invest less in children.
Therefore, men should be expected to be less discriminating and more
aggressive in competing for females.
It was an elegant, powerful application of evolutionary theory to the
mating game. The evolutionary psychologists of the 1980s and ’90s built
on Mr. Trivers’s theory to explain a wide array of stereotypical gender
differences in mating.
In 1993, David M. Buss and David P. Schmitt used parental investment
theory to explain why men should be expected to “devote a larger
proportion of their total mating effort to short-term mating.” Because
men invested less time and effort in their offspring, they evolved
toward promiscuity, while women evolved away from it. Promiscuity, the
researchers hypothesized, would have been more damaging to the female
reputation than to the male reputation. If a man mated with a
promiscuous woman, he would never be able to ensure his paternity. Men,
on the other hand, could potentially enhance their status by pursuing a
short-term mating strategy. (Think Kennedy, Clinton, Spitzer, Letterman
and so forth. My space is limited.)
One of the earliest critics of this kind of thinking was Stephen Jay
Gould. He wrote in 1997 that parental investment theory “will not
explain the full panoply of supposed sexual differences so dear to pop
psychology.” Mr. Gould felt that the field had become overrun with
“ultra-Darwinians,” and that evolutionary psychology would be a more
fruitful science if it didn’t limit itself “to the blinkered view” that
evolutionary explanations accounted for every difference.
But if evolution didn’t determine human behavior, what did? The most
common explanation is the effect of cultural norms. That, for instance,
society tends to view promiscuous men as normal and promiscuous women as
troubled outliers, or that our “social script” requires men to approach
women while the pickier women do the selecting. Over the past decade,
sociocultural explanations have gained steam.
Take the question of promiscuity. Everyone has always assumed — and
early research had shown — that women desired fewer sexual partners over
a lifetime than men. But in 2003, two behavioral psychologists, Michele
G. Alexander and Terri D. Fisher, published the results of a study that
used a “bogus pipeline” — a fake lie detector. When asked about actual
sexual partners, rather than just theoretical desires, the participants
who were not attached to the fake lie detector displayed typical gender
differences. Men reported having had more sexual partners than women.
But when participants believed that lies about their sexual history
would be revealed by the fake lie detector, gender differences in
reported sexual partners vanished. In fact, women reported slightly more
sexual partners (a mean of 4.4) than did men (a mean of 4.0).
In 2009, another long-assumed gender difference in mating — that women
are choosier than men — also came under siege. In speed dating, as in
life, the social norm instructs women to sit in one place, waiting to be
approached, while the men rotate tables. But in one study of
speed-dating behavior, the evolutionary psychologists Eli J. Finkel and
Paul W. Eastwick switched the “rotator” role. The men remained seated
and the women rotated. By manipulating this component of the gender
script, the researchers discovered that women became less selective —
they behaved more like stereotypical men — while men were more selective
and behaved more like stereotypical women. The mere act of physically
approaching a potential romantic partner, they argued, engendered more
favorable assessments of that person.
Recently, a third pillar appeared to fall. To back up the assumption
that an enormous gap exists between men’s and women’s attitudes toward
casual sex, evolutionary psychologists typically cite a classic study
published in 1989. Men and women on a college campus were approached in
public and propositioned with offers of casual sex by “confederates” who
worked for the study. The confederate would say: “I have been noticing
you around campus and I find you to be very attractive.” The confederate
would then ask one of three questions: (1) “Would you go out with me
tonight?” (2) “Would you come over to my apartment tonight?” or (3)
“Would you go to bed with me tonight?”
Roughly equal numbers of men and women agreed to the date. But women
were much less likely to agree to go to the confederate’s apartment. As
for going to bed with the confederate, zero women said yes, while about
70 percent of males agreed.
Those results seemed definitive — until a few years ago, when Terri D.
Conley, a psychologist at the University of Michigan, set out to
re-examine what she calls “one of the largest documented sexuality
gender differences,” that men have a greater interest in casual sex than
women.
Ms. Conley found the methodology of the 1989 paper to be less than
ideal. “No one really comes up to you in the middle of the quad and
asks, ‘Will you have sex with me?’ ” she told me recently. “So there
needs to be a context for it. If you ask people what they would do in a
specific situation, that’s a far more accurate way of getting
responses.” In her study, when men and women considered offers of casual
sex from famous people, or offers from close friends whom they were
told were good in bed, the gender differences in acceptance of
casual-sex proposals evaporated nearly to zero.
In light of this new research, will Darwinians consider revising their
theories to reflect the possibility that our mating behavior is less
hard-wired than they had believed?
Probably not. In an article responding to the new studies last year, Mr.
Schmitt, a leading voice among hard-line Darwinians, ceded no ground.
Addressing Ms. Conley’s finding that women were more likely to agree to
casual sex with a celebrity, Mr. Schmitt argued that this resulted from
“women’s (but not men’s) short-term mating psychology being specially
designed to obtain good genes from physically attractive short-term
partners.” He continued: “When women’s short-term-mating aim is
activated (perhaps, temporarily, because of, e.g., high-fertility
ovulatory status or desire for an extramarital affair, or more
chronically, because of , e.g., a female-biased local sex ratio or a
history of insecure parent-child attachment), they appear to express
relatively focused desires for genetic traits in ‘sexy men’ that would
biologically benefit women when short-term mating.”
In other words: Nothing new here, it’s all evolution.
Steven Pinker, the Harvard psychologist and popular author, also backs
the Darwinians, whom he says still have the weight of evidence on their
side. “A study which shows you can push some phenomenon around a bit at
the margins,” he wrote to me in an e-mail, “is of dubious relevance to
whether the phenomenon exists.”
But the fact that some gender differences can be manipulated, if not
eliminated, by controlling for cultural norms suggests that the
explanatory power of evolution can’t sustain itself when applied to
mating behavior. This wouldn’t be the first time we’ve pushed these
theories too far. How many stereotypical racial and ethnic differences,
once declared evolutionarily determined under the banner of science,
have been revealed instead as vestiges of power dynamics from earlier
societies?
Citing the speed-dating study, Mr. Pinker added, “The only reason this
flawed paper was published was that it challenged an evolutionary
hypothesis ... in particular a sex difference — as the Larry Summers
incident shows, claims about sex differences are still politically
inflammatory in the academy.” Here, he was referring to the much
criticized 2005 comments Mr. Summers made when he was Harvard’s
president suggesting that women’s underrepresentation in science and
engineering was attributable not to socialization but to “different
availability of aptitude at the high end.”
Perhaps these phenomena exist. Perhaps men do, over all, pursue more
short-term mating. But given new research, continued rigid reliance on
evolution as an explanation seems to risk elevating a limited guide to
teleological status — a way of thinking that scientists should abhor.
“Some sexual features are deeply rooted in evolutionary heritage, such
as the sex response and how quickly it takes men and women to become
aroused,” said Paul Eastwick, a co-author of the speed-dating study.
“However, if you’re looking at features such as how men and women
regulate themselves in society to achieve specific goals, I believe
those features are unlikely to have evolved sex differences. I consider
myself an evolutionary psychologist. But many evolutionary psychologists
don’t think this way. They think these features are getting shaped and
honed by natural selection all the time.” How far does Darwin go in
explaining human behavior?
No comments:
Post a Comment